Institute for Language &
Education Policy
Anti-Bilingual
Initiative
"Babel" in Schools
Bilingual Ballots
Bilingual Education
Bilingual
Research
Canards
Demographic Change
Endangered Languages
English Only
English Plus
Language Rights
Language Legislation
Life After Prop. 227
Louisiana
Muhlenberg Legend
"Multilingual Government"
National Identity
Official English
Opinion Polls
Puerto Rico
Research
Controversy
Recent Monographs
Jim Cummins, Beyond Adversarial Discourse (1998)
Jay Greene, A Meta-Analysis of the Effectiveness of Bilingual Education
(1998)
Stephen Krashen, A Note on Greene's "Meta-Analysis of the Effectiveness
of Bilingual Education"(1998)
Kenji Hakuta, Memorandum on Read Institute press release on NRC Report
(1997)
Diane August and Kenji Hakuta,
Letter to Rosalie Porter (1997)
|
Issues in U.S. Language Policy
Bilingual Education
Recent Research
For many Americans, bilingual education seems to defy common sense
– not to mention the Melting Pot tradition. They ask:
- If non-English-speaking students are isolated in foreign-language
classrooms, how are they ever going to learn English, the key to upward
mobility?
- What was wrong with the old "sink or swim" method that worked for
generations of earlier immigrants?
- Isn't bilingual education just another example of "political correctness"
run amok – the inability to say no to a vociferous ethnic lobby?
Some English Only advocates go further, arguing
that even if bilingual education is effective – which they doubt – it's
still a bad idea for the country because bilingualism threatens to sap our
sense of national identity and divide us along ethnic lines. They fear that
any government recognition of minority languages "sends the wrong message"
to immigrants, encouraging them to believe they can live in the U.S.A. without
learning English or conforming to "American" ways.
Such complaints have made bilingual education a target of political
attacks. One of the most serious to date is now under way in California,
a ballot
initiative that would mandate English-only instruction for all children
until they become fully proficient in English.
No doubt many of the objections to bilingual education are lodged
in good faith. Others reflect ethnic stereotypes or class biases. Sad to say,
they all reflect a pervasive ignorance about how bilingual education works,
how second languages are acquired, and how the nation has responded to non-English-speaking
groups in the past.
Reinforcing popular fallacies requires
less space than deconstructing them. That's why my writing on these issues
grew from a handful of newspaper articles into a 310-page book, Bilingual Education: History, Politics, Theory, and Practice
(3rd ed., 1995). Nevertheless, a few points:
Science is often counterintuitive. Its breakthroughs tend to upset common-sense
notions, not to mention cherished myths. Linguistics is no exception. In fact,
it invites more than its share of opposition from nonspecialists – witness
the "Ebonics" controversy – because
most people feel like experts when it comes to language. Our reactions are
often visceral. Perhaps that's because our speech defines us ethnically, socially,
and intellectually. It's tied up with a sense of who we are – and who we
are not – evoking some of our deepest emotions.
What once seemed obvious about bilingualism – for example, that it handicaps
children's cognitive growth – has usually proved unfounded. Since the 1960s,
research has shown that multiple language skills do not confuse the mind.
Quite the contrary: when well-developed, they seem to provide cognitive advantages,
although such effects are complex and difficult to measure (Hakuta 1986).
Another discredited notion is that children will "pick up" a second language
rapidly if "totally immersed" in it. For generations, this philosophy served
to justify policies of educational neglect – assigning minority students to
regular classrooms, with no special help in overcoming language barriers.
Disproportionate numbers failed and dropped out of school as a result. The
sink-or-swim approach was ruled illegal by the U.S. Supreme Court in Lau v. Nichols (1974).
Research has shown that the quality – not the quantity – of English exposure
is the major factor in English acquisition. That is, the second-language
input must be comprehensible(Krashen 1996).
Otherwise, it's just noise.
English as a second language (ESL) is best taught in natural situations,
with the second language used in meaningful contexts rather than in repetitious
drills of grammar and vocabulary. One variant of ESL, known as "sheltered
subject-matter instruction," adapts lessons to students' level of English
proficiency. This approach is common in bilingual education programs, coordinated
with lessons in students' native language.
Native-language instruction also helps to make English comprehensible,
by providing contextual knowledge that aids in understanding. When children
already know something about dinosaurs, a lesson on the subject will make
more sense when instruction shifts to English. Not only will they learn more
about dinosaurs; they will also acquire more English.
The same principle applies when it comes to acquiring literacy. Teaching
in the native language can facilitate the process, as the linguist Stephen
Krashen (1996) explains:
- We learn to read by reading, by making sense of what we see on the
page. ...
- If we learn to read by reading, it will be much easier to learn to
read in a language we already understand.
- Once you can read, you can read. The ability to read transfers across
languages.
"Language is not a unitary skill, but a complex configuration of abilities"
(Hakuta and Snow 1986). Social communication skills –
a.k.a. playground English – should not be confused with academic
English, the cognitively demanding language that children need to succeed
in school. While playground English tends to be acquired rapidly by most children,
academic English is typically acquired over a longer period. This explains why it takes second-language learners five to seven years, on average, to catch up with English-proficient peers on tests given in English
(Cummins 1989).
Research on the effectiveness of bilingual education remains in dispute,
because program evaluation studies – featuring appropriate comparison
groups and random assignment of subjects or controls for pre-existing differences
– are extremely difficult to design. Moreover, there is considerable
variation among the pedagogies, schools, students, and communities being compared.
While numerous studies have documented the benefits of bilingual programs,
much of this research has faced methodological criticisms – as noted
by an expert panel of the National Research Council
(August and Hakuta 1997a).
Certain critics of bilingual education have interpreted the NRC report
to mean that, despite a generation of research, "there is no evidence that
there will be long-term advantages or disadvantages to teaching limited-English
students in the native language" (Glenn 1997). This conclusion
– widely circulated by the so-called READ Institute –
has been rejected by the NRC study directors. To the
contrary, they say, the expert panel concluded that "a great deal has been
learned from the research that has been conducted on English language learners."
Moreover, there are "empirical results . . . support[ing] the theory underlying
native language instruction" (August and Hakuta 1997b).
According to the panel's chairman, the "attempt by READ to place its own political
spin" on the report hardly advances the cause of objective research (Hakuta 1997).
Other critics continue to deny that such empirical support exists. A recent
"review of the literature" (Rossell and Baker 1996) reports
that bilingual education is inferior to English-only programs of all kinds,
including sink-or-swim. Yet these conclusions owe more to the manipulation
of program labels than to student performance in the classroom. Critiques
of Rossell and Baker by Cummins (1998) and Krashen (1996) show that, among other distortions, the
researchers rely heavily on studies of French immersion in Canada –
bilingual or trilingual approaches that they portray as monolingual "immersion"
or "submersion" models. Meanwhile, a meta-analysis
of the same body of research reviewed by the critics, but using a more rigorous
methodology, found quite different results: a significant edge for bilingual
education (Greene 1998).
The most sophisticated evaluation study to date
– a four-year, longitudinal study of 2,000 Spanish-speaking students
in five states – found that "late-exit," developmental bilingual
programs proved superior to "early-exit," transitional bilingual
and English-only immersionprograms (Ramírez
et al. 1991). That is, in programs that stressed native-language
skills, students' growth in English reading and mathematics continued to
increase long after it had leveled off among their peers in the other programs.
While this study has been praised by many, others have rejected the comparison
as invalid because all three programs were not tested in the same school
districts.
Nevertheless, a consensus of applied linguists recognizes that the following
propositions have strong empirical support:
- Native-language instruction does not retard the acquisition of English.
- Well-developed skills in the native language are associated with
high levels of academic achievement.
- Bilingualism is a valuable skill, for individuals and for the country.
Bilingual education was adopted by many local school districts in the 1960s
and 1970s to remedy practices that had denied language minorities an equal
educational opportunity. Yet it was hardly a new invention designed to replace
the Melting Pot with the Salad Bowl or some other model of ethnic pluralism.
There is a long bilingual tradition in the U.S.A.,
in which minority-language schooling has played a central, albeit largely
forgotten, role.
References
August, Diane, and Hakuta, Kenji, eds. 1997a.
Improving Schooling for Language-Minority
Children: A Research Agenda. Washington,
D.C.: National Academy Press.
August, Diane, and Hakuta, Kenji. 1997b. Letter to Rosalie Porter, READ Institute.
Cummins, Jim. 1998. Beyond Adversarial Discourse: Searching for Common Ground
in the Education of Bilingual Students. Presentation to the California
State Board of Education, Feb. 9, 1998.
Cummins, Jim. 1989. Empowering Minority Students. Sacramento,
Calif.: California Association for Bilingual Education.
Glenn, Charles L. 1997. What Does The National Research Council
Study Tell Us About Educating Language Minority Children? Amherst,
Mass.: READ Institute.
Greene, Jay. 1998. A Meta-Analysis of the Effectiveness of Bilingual Education.
Claremont, Calif.: Tomás Rivera Center.
Hakuta, Kenji. 1997. Memorandum
on Read Institute press release on NRC Report.
Hakuta, Kenji. 1986. Mirror of Language:
The Debate on Bilingualism. New York: Basic Books.
Hakuta, Kenji, and Snow, Catherine. 1986. "The
Role of Research in Policy Decisions about Bilingual Education." NABE
News 9, no. 3 (Spring): 1, 18-21.
Krashen, Stephen D. 1996. Under Attack:
The Case Against Bilingual Education. Culver City, Calif.: Language
Education Associates.
Ramírez, J. David; Yuen, Sandra D.; and
Ramey, Dena R. 1991. Final Report: Longitudinal Study of Structured
Immersion Strategy, Early-Exit, and Late-Exit Transitional Bilingual Education
Programs for Language-Minority Children. San Mateo, Calif.: Aguirre
International.
Rossell, Christine, and Baker, Keith. 1996. The
effectiveness of bilingual education. Research in the Teaching of English,30,pp.7-74.
Copyright © 1998 by
James Crawford. Permission is hereby granted to reproduce this page for free,
noncommercial distribution, provided that credit is given and this notice
is included. Requests for permission to reproduce in any other form should
be emailed to jwcrawford@compuserve.com.
But before writing, please read my permissions FAQ.
|